Spina Bifida Family Support
"Families Helping Families"
Cognitive status of young adults with spina bifida
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology December 2003
The availability of new medical treatment has meant that the life expectancy
of patients with spina bifida (SB) has increased and a significant population of
young adults with SB has come into existence (Lorber 1971, Ouden et al. 1996,
McDonnell and McCann 2000, Bowman et al. 2001). As a result, a growing number of
young adults with SB are facing challenges with respect to education, vocation,
housing, and relationships. In the terms of the International Classification of
Functioning, Disability and Health (World Health Organization 2001),
restrictions in participation in these domains are not explained by the severity
of neurological impairments, rather, indirect effects have been suggested.
Neurological impairments, such as the type of SB and hydrocephalus (HC; Loomis
et al. 1994), limit neuropsychological (cognitive and affective) functions, and
the neuropsychological functions in turn restrict participation (Hommeyer et al.
1999). This study examines the relation between neurological parameters and
cognition.
In children with SB under 16 years of age the presence of HC has been identified
as the major illness parameter that affects the level of cognitive functioning
(Wills 1993). Illness parameters that are related to HC (i.e. the necessity of
shunting and number of shunt revisions, encephalitis, epileptic seizures, and
additional structural abnormalities of the central nervous system) also affect
the level of cognitive functioning (Spain 1974, Tew and Laurence 1975, Dennis et
al. 1981, Shaffer et al. 1985, Wills et al. 1990, Friedrich et al. 1991, Wills
1993, Kokkonen et al. 1994, Snow et al. 1994, Holler et al. 1995, Fletcher et
al. 1996, Bier et al. 1997, Fletcher et al. 1997, Hunt et al. 1999). Some
studies have found that the level of the lesion is related to cognitive
functioning (Shaffer et al. 1985, Wills 1993, Bier et al. 1997). Among
non-illness parameters, socioeconomic status contributes to differences in the
cognitive and social outcome of patients with SB (Bier et al. 1997).
Few studies have addressed the cognitive outcome of persons with SB over the age
of 16 years (Friedrich et al. 1993, Loomis et al. 1994, Snow et al. 1994, West
et al. 1995, Bier et al. 1997, Dise and Lohr 1998, Mataro et al. 2000). The
limitations of these studies are either a selection bias (e.g. participants
having been included on the basis of intelligence or of HC), a rather wide age
range, or an uncertainty about the presence of HC in the participants. As a
result, the proportion of the population of young adults with HC with
impairment(s) in intelligence, cognitive flexibility, problem-solving ability,
and processing efficiency remains to be established. Insight into the cognitive
status and factors affecting the cognitive status of this group when they reach
young adulthood will provide paediatricians with a better understanding of the
results of treatment that has been given to them.
The purpose of this study was to assess the cognitive status of young adults
with SB, including the often-neglected group with SB occulta. In order to
explore possible effects of primary neurological parameters, i.e. type of SB (occulta
vs aperta) and presence or absence of HC, we divided the group into three
subgroups and compared the cognitive data obtained from the three groups.
Furthermore, we investigated and compared the effects of secondary parameters
(complications of HC) and of associated pathology (epileptic seizures and
additional brain malformations). Level of lesion, being associated with both
types of SB and HC, was not entered as a separate variable.
Method
This multicentre study was part of the Adolescents with Spina Bifida in the
Netherlands (ASPINE) project, a cross-sectional study of physical and cognitive
abilities, health care, participation in society, and life satisfaction of young
people with SB. The ethics and research committees of the participating
institutions have approved ASPINE.
PARTICIPANTS
To be included, participants had to have SB aperta or occulta (International
Classification of Diseases, Ninth revision, Classification of Mental Disorders
codes 741 and 756.17 respectively, World Health Organization 1979), be aged
between 16 and 25 years, and have sufficient command of the Dutch language.
Participants were recruited by 11 of the 12 Dutch Spina Bifida Teams, in
coordination with the Dutch Spina Bifida Patients Association, organizations for
sheltered homes, and rehabilitation centres. Written invitations to participate
were signed by the patient's physicians or by the management of the sheltered
homes. Written informed consent was obtained from the participants or their
parents, where applicable.
Invitations were sent to 350 prospective participants which yielded 181 (52%)
positive responses. Known causes of non-participation were unknown address (29%)
and time limitations of the invitee (29%). Participants did not differ from
non-participants in age, sex, type of SB, level of lesion, or being shunted for
HC.
Ten of the 181 individuals who were willing to participate were only interviewed
by telephone; they were not included in the present study. Two participants with
comorbidity that could independently induce serious physical and/or cognitive
impairments were excluded: one had serious heart disease and one had a
chromosome disorder. Participants with pathology associated with SB were not
excluded from the analysis, apart from one participant who could not be examined
neuropsychologically because of blindness which was acquired after having been
shunted for HC early in life. Data for 168 participants were included for the
analysis.
MATERIALS
The headings under which the tests are listed below represent the measurement
pretensions of the tests for the healthy population. Tests were administered
according to their respective manuals.
General intelligence
The Standard Progressive Matrices (Raven 1996, Raven et al. 1998) is a multiple
choice test requiring accuracy of discrimination and evaluation of logical
relations in visual displays. It provides an index of fluid intelligence (Heaton
et al. 1986). We used a 20-minute time limit for the results of which recent
Dutch normative scores are available (Bouma et al. 1996). The dependent variable
is the number of adequate solutions, corrected for age and sex, and converted to
an IQ score. Mean IQ score of the healthy population is 100 (SD 15).
Memory and verbal learning
The Wechsler Memory Scale (Wechsler 1974) provides a global measure of memory.
The sub tests are: Personal and Current Information, Orientation to Time and
Place, Mental Control, Logical Memory, Digit Span, Visual Reproduction, and
Associative Learning. In this study only the age-corrected memory quotient (MQ;
population mean 100; SD 15) was used as the dependent variable.
The Verbal Learning Test (VLT; Mulder et al. 1996) is a recent Dutch version of
the California Verbal Learning Test (Delis et al. 1987). Participants are orally
presented with a 'shopping list' (list A) consisting of 16 common nouns drawn
from four semantic categories (fruits, clothing, tools, and spices). The items
are presented five times (trials) in identical order. In each trial participants
recall as many nouns as they can think of. After list A, a second (interfering)
list is presented, of which the participant is again asked to recall as many
nouns as possible. This is followed by a free and cued recall of list A. In the
latter, the participant is provided with the semantic categories of list A.
After 20 minutes filled with distracting tests, free and cued recall of list A
is again determined. Finally, the participant is asked to identify the 16 nouns
of list A among 28 distracting nouns. Thanks to elaborate psychometric studies
in the Dutch population (Mulder et al. 1996), learning indices can be compared
with population normative values. The indices are corrected for age and sex and
normative scores are calculated. The normative scores (z scores multiplied by
two), have a mean of 0 (SD 2). For the purpose of this study, we inspected the
normative scores and selected the three most informative learning parameters:
Total Recall of list A (the total number of recalled items across five trials),
Learning slope (the rate of learning of additional items across five trials),
and Consolidation (the ability to remember learnt items over a longer period of
time).
Executive functioning
In the Wisconsin Modified Card Sorting Test (Nelson 1976), four stimulus cards are presented depicting one of four different shapes, which also differ in colour and number. The response cards have one of three categories - shape, colour, or number - in common with the standard stimuli but none of the cards are identical to the standard. The participant is asked to sort the cards and is informed that the examiner will tell them whether their choice was correct or not. After six correct responses the participant is asked to sort differently. The ability to maintain and to shift a cognitive set is inferred from the number of categories found and applied correctly; we scored the number of correct categories. The maximum is 6, which is also the expected number in individuals without cognitive impairment.
The Trail Making Test (TMT; Reitan and Wolfson 1996) is a paper and pencil test. Part A requires the participant to connect 25 circles, placed randomly on an A4 sheet of paper and numbered 1 to 25, in sequential order. In part B, 13 of the 25 circles are numbered from 1 to 13 and 12 contain one of the first 12 letters of the alphabet. The participant has to connect the numbers and letters as quickly as they can, alternating the sequences. The time to complete the task is recorded for both parts. We took the time (in seconds) for completing part B and the time difference between part B and part A (B-A) as measures of divided attention (Corrigan and Hinkeldey 1987).
Word production according to lexical rules (UNKA test; Jennekens-Schinkel et al. 1990) addresses the interface of memory, language, and behavioural regulation. The participant is asked to generate as many words as they can think of that begin with the prescribed letters (U, N, K, A). Production time is 60 seconds per letter. The dependent variable is the total number of correct words. Data on the performance of the typically developing population is available (A Jennekens-Schinkel, personal communication 2001).
Reaction time
Reaction time was measured with the Time Tapper test (Fetrics, Holland VOF). Participants were asked to react as quickly as possible to stimuli appearing on a panel in front of them by lifting their finger from the rest button, moving to and pressing the reaction button, and then returning to the rest button. In the two simple types, single stimuli were presented (respectively a green light and a tone) and the participant had to react to every stimulus. The third and fourth types were of the 'go-no-go' type. Stimuli were presented singly or in combination and the participant had to react only to a target subset (either of an intra- or of an intermodal nature). The dependent variables were the median decision times (numbers of milliseconds between stimulus presentation and lifting the finger from the rest button) and the median motor times (numbers of milliseconds between lifting the finger from the rest button to touching the reaction button) averaged across the four conditions.
DATA COLLECTION
Medical records were examined according to a fixed protocol and participants underwent a physical examination by a clinician. They were interviewed in a semi-structured way and were assessed by a neuropsychologist. Data were gathered with respect to type of SB, level of lesion (defined as the lowest completely unimpaired level on both sides measured with sensitivity to pin prick and light touch), HC, number of shunt revisions, ventriculitis, epilepsy, corpus callosum malformation, cerebral bleeding or ischaemia, total number of surgical interventions other then shunting and shunt revisions, ambulation (Hoffer et al. 1973), incontinence (defined as wetting at least once a month), living arrangement, and educational/vocational status.
The total examination was conducted within 3½ hours. Participants were randomly picked for examination, so that half of the participants started with the physical examination and the other half started with the neuropsychological examination. The order of the neuropsychological tests was fixed and was ordered in such a way that there would be no interference between the different tests. The 1 hour 30 minute neuropsychological examination included a short break. For logistical reasons, participants were split randomly into two groups. Both groups were administered the tests of intelligence, memory, and executive function. Additionally, one group was administered the VLT and the other group the reaction speed tasks. Twenty-five participants did not complete the test battery, mostly because of lack of time. For these participants the missing values were substituted by the mean score of the total sample on that particular test.
DATA ANALYSIS
Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS for Windows (version 10.0.7). Illness characteristics and sex of the three clinical groups of participants were compared using the [chi]^sup 2^ test. Age was compared with analysis of variance (ANOVA).
Because group size was small for some variables, and because some variables violated the assumption of normality, cognition and speed of the three clinical groups were compared by means of ANOVA and, if appropriate, by the non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis test. Statistically significant group differences were analyzed post hoc using the independent samples t-test with a Bonferroni correction (alpha/number of tests) and, if appropriate, the Mann-Whitney U test. Alpha was set at 0.05. Parametric and non-parametric analyses did not yield any different results, therefore we only report the parametric data.
For case finding, the clinical significance of poor performances was denned by setting a cut-off point and considering scores from 2SD worse than the population mean of the variable as 'deviation scores' in the IQ, MQ, VLT, UNKA tests. In the absence of suitable population data, the reference value was assigned to the group with SB occulta for the TMT-B, TMT-(B-A), and reaction speed measures. For the Wisconsin Modified Card Sorting Test, the clinically significant cut-off point of five categories was used because failing to find all six categories is pathognomonic for impairment. Between the three clinical groups, group differences in the proportions of participants who obtained deviation scores were compared using the [chi]^sup 2^ test. Furthermore, we summed the deviation scores to a 'total cognitive deviation' score, in order to explore the range (0 to 6) across cognitive domains.
For analysis of the effect of number of drain revisions and of associated pathology on the cognitive status within the group of persons with SB aperta with HC (AHC+), we used the cognitive measure that correlated most strongly with all other cognitive measures, i.e. IQ. Statistical analyses were performed using the independent samples t-test.
Results
Demographic and neurological characteristics of the groups of persons with SB occulta, SB aperta without HC (AHC-), and SB aperta with HC (AHC+) arc summarized in Table 1. Sixty percent of the participants were female. The AHC- had a significantly smaller proportion of females than the two other groups (p=0.043). Mean age of all participants was 20 years 9 months; age was not significantly different between the groups. All AHC+ participants had been shunted for HC early in life. The majority (63%) among them having had two or more shunt revisions. Early in life, one of 37 participants in the SB occulta group and five of 20 participants with AHC- had had transient signs of raised intracranial pressure which had never required shunting. As they did not differ from those without HC, these six participants with presumably compensated HC were treated as non-hydrocephalic participants for the analyses of cognition and reaction speed. The distribution of levels of lesion was similar for persons with SB occulta and AHC-. Among persons with AHC+ the proportion of high level lesions was significantly larger (p<0.001) than among persons without HC. The measure of collinearity (Cramer's [Phi]) was 0.406 between type of SB and level of lesion, 0.742 between type of SB and HC, and 0.585 between level of lesion and HC. Pathology associated with SB, i.e. encephalitis, epilepsy, corpus callosum malformation, cerebral bleeding, and ischaemia of the brain, were uncommon and not restricted to persons with AHC+. Persons with AHC+ had undergone more surgical interventions than persons with SB occulta (p=0.002). Persons with HC were more often wheelchair dependent and incontinent than those without HC (p<0.001). Also, they lived more often in sheltered homes, were more often unemployed or had sheltered work, and had received more special education (p<0.001).
ANOVA revealed significant differences between
the groups of persons with SB occulta, AHC, and AHC+ on all but one of the
cognitive and speed tasks (Table II, Fig. 1). Post-hoc analysis showed no
significant difference between the groups with SB occulta and AHC-. Mean scores
of individuals with AHC+ on all but one test were significantly worse (p<0.01)
than of both SB occulta and AHC-. Figure 1 clearly shows that cognitive
functioning of persons with SB occulta and AHC- was similar and near to normal
and that cognitive functioning of persons with AHC+ was below average in most
domains. Specifically, for AHC+ individuals' IQ ranged from 55 to 117, whereas
for persons with AHC-, IQ ranged from 69 to 117.
The majority of deviation scores (Table III, Fig. 1) were obtained by The AHC+
participants: about one-fifth had an IQ or MQ below 70. Between one-third and
one-half of AHC+ participants had a deviation score on the VLT. Almost half
failed on the Wisconsin Modified Card Sorting Test. A quarter deviated on TMT-B;
when corrected for completion time of part A, this percentage dropped to 12%.
Only 7% had a deviation score on the UNKA. As far as reaction speed was
concerned, deviating decision times were found in 44% of The AHC+ participants
and deviating motor times in one quarter of AHC+ participants. Participants of
both other groups obtained considerably fewer deviation scores. Four
participants with SB occulta had deviation scores in consolidation (VLT). Among
those with AHC-, three of 20 participants had a deviation score on the Wisconsin
Modified Card Sorting Test and three of 12 had a deviation score on both
decision time and motor time of the reaction speed task. When exploring
generality versus specificity of the deviations, six persons with AHC- had one
deviation score and none had 2 or more deviation scores. Similar to persons with
AHC-, of the persons with SB occulta, 24% had one or more deviation scores and
one person had 2 deviation scores. In contrast, 79% of the persons with AHC+ had
one or more deviation scores, 44% had 2 or more deviation scores, and 15% had a
more general deviation with 4 or more deviation scores.
IQ significantly correlated with all but one of the other cognitive scores.
Pearson's r ranged from 0.21 to 0.73. The majority of the correlations (7 of 10)
were higher than 0.40. IQ was found to be the most robust measure of general
cognitive status. Within the group with AHC+ , mean IQ decreased with increasing
number of shunt revisions: 0 or 1 revision (mean IQ=86), 2 to 4 revisions (mean
IQ=86), and 5 or more revisions (mean IQ=77). Post-hoc analysis showed the
difference between the groups with no or one shunt revision and with five or
more shunt revisions to be significant (p<0.0167).
As far as associated pathology was concerned, when grouping together all AHC+
participants with associated pathology and comparing this group (n=24) with the
group without associated pathology, a statistically significant IQ difference
appeared: former group mean IQ 75 versus mean IQ 85 for the latter group
(p=0.003). Further analyses showed that in particular, corpus callosum
malformation (four participants of seven vs 17% of those without corpus callosum
malformation) with epilepsy (six participants of 11 vs 16% of those without
epilepsy) was related to IQs below 70. Of the five persons with associated
pathology but without HC, the one who had had encephalitis had a low-average IQ
of 84. IQs of the other four individuals were average.
Discussion
The purpose of the present study was to assess the cognitive status of young
adults with SB aperta or occulta, a sparsely studied issue which is, however,
relevant to establishing the effects of SB and HC on cognitive status later in
life and for understanding psychosocial outcome (Hommeyer et al. 1999).
Three participant groups were distinguished: SB occulta, AHC+, and AHC-. Our
results indicate that the cognitive status of persons with AHC+ is in the
below-average range when compared with the population mean. Moreover, the
performances of persons with SB occulta and AHC- are similar to those in the
typically developing population. The conclusion is that the presence of HC is
the main culprit for a poor cognitive status and SB aperta, per se, has no
negative effect.
As for the group of persons with SB occulta, the conclusion that the cognitive
abilities of persons in this group are normal confirms the findings of Friedrich
et al. (1993) in a previous study using a small number (n=10) of patients with
lipomyelomeningocele. However, it should be noted that this study included three
persons with HC. Also in our study, the group of participants with SB occulta
did not consist only of persons with lipomyelomeningocele but also included
other types of SB occulta. We now conclude that in the wider population of
individuals with SB occulta, cognitive abilities are similar to those of the
general population.
As mentioned earlier, the group with AHC- performed similarly to individuals
with SB occulta on the cognitive tests. Few deviation scores were found and none
of the persons with AHC- had 2 or more deviation scores. Together with Friedrich
et al. (1991), we conclude that those with AHC- do not have serious cognitive
problems. Considering the small number of participants in this subgroup, further
research is warranted.
The group of individuals with AHC+ scored below average on most cognitive tasks.
The findings are roughly similar to those described for children (Mapstone et
al. 1984, Wills et al. 1990, Wills 1993, Yeates et al. 1995, Fletcher et al.
1997, Snow 1999) and older persons (West et al. 1995, Bier et al. 1997, Dise and
Lohr 1998, Hommet et al. 1999). Differences in operational methods hamper
specific comparisons. Although persons with AHC+ scored below average as a
group, it should be noted that half of the AHC+ participants had a (near to)
normal cognitive status. This finding acids to the available literature on the
heterogeneity of those with SB (Wills 1993, Snow et al. 1994, West et al. 1995)
supporting the notion that the prototypical SB patient is an untenable
abstraction.
Cognitive status is not influenced solely by the presence of HC. Corpus callosum
malformation and epilepsy appear to be negatively related to intelligence. Also,
in line with previous findings (Holler et al. 1995) but contradicting recent
findings (Ralph et al. 2000), within the group of persons with HC, having had
five or more shunt revisions had a negative effect on IQ. It should be noted
that in this study both a higher level of lesion and having associated pathology
is positively related to the number of shunt revisions (data not shown). It may
well be that the associated pathology is the major disruptive factor in
cognition. Epilepsy was rare in the present study and its effect could not be
assessed independently, but the finding of lower IQ in those with associated
pathology (including epilepsy) may be understood on the same basis as the
negative effect of seizures (Ralph et al. 2000).
Three features of the present study have to be mentioned. Every effort was made
to approach all those with SB, including those with SB occulta, and to examine
every individual we were able to trace. Non-response analysis showed that the
participant group was similar to the non-participant group on important
demographic and illness characteristics. Therefore, we feel that our findings
can be generalized over the population of young adults with SB in the
Netherlands. Of course, only those patients were included who were diagnosed
with SB occulta at birth or later on in life when the defect caused physical
complaints. There certainly are many persons with undiscovered SB occulta. This
selection bias will probably make little difference to cognitive outcome, in
which we found the full breadth of normality in this group.
The second feature to be mentioned is that we did not exclude patients with an
IQ below 70. We insisted on including these participants for the reason that
learning disability* is a possible outcome of the neural tube defect. Excluding
participants with learning disability would possibly lead to a too optimistic
view of cognitive status.
The third feature is the application of an arbitrary but very strict rule for
determining deviation scores. We wanted to minimize the chance of overestimating
the true state of affairs regarding cognitive deficits. Our results may,
therefore, even underestimate the number of persons with a cognitive deficit.
One problem with our study was that logistic reasons necessitated the population
to be randomly split into two groups which were administered a core battery of
tests (assessing intelligence, memory, and executive functioning) and part of
the remaining tests (verbal learning or reaction speed). The missing values
restrained the analysis of the group data and prohibited statistical analysis of
the deviation sum score. On some occasions participants could not complete all
of the assessment. By substituting a missing value on a particular test by the
mean score of the total sample we tried to avoid biasing the results in favour
of detecting deviating scores in the clinical subgroups.
A final shortcoming of this study was that reference scores for a healthy,
age-appropriate population were not available for all tests. Based on the
clinical intuition that patients with SB occulta are not different from the
typically developing population in major aspects of cognitive functioning, we
took the mean score and standard deviation of the occulta group as a reference.
Our choice was supported by the data on tests for which normative scores are
available.
Conclusion
Our results suggest that the cognitive status of young adults with SB occulta
and AHC- is normal. Almost 50% of young adults with AHC+ have a cognitive
impairment of some sort. Cognitive status appears to be negatively influenced by
multiple shunt revisions and, particularly, by pathology associated with HC.
Future research should concentrate on a more detailed examination of cognition
and should include a more qualitative analysis of the performance, for instance
by a comprehensive error analysis. Further research should also concentrate on
the consequences of cognitive impairments for education, vocation, and social
participation.